The Science of Breakups: Why Calling Things Off Is Easier Said Than Done - Huffington Post


Posted:



Relationships frequently fall apart due to irreconcilable incompatibilities. Sometimes these incompatibilities are so large that they seem like they should have been obvious from the start (e.g., one person wants children, the other partner doesn't; one person is deeply religious, the other isn't). Why don't such dealbreakers prevent relationships from getting off the ground in the first place? Why do people so frequently wind up with incompatible romantic partners?


Some time ago, I wrote a post on Science of Relationships about how single people can readily call to mind all of the traits and features that they are looking for in a mate, yet these preferences seem to go right out the window when people make real-life dating decisions. Research consistently shows that what people say they want in a partner has virtually no bearing on who they actually choose to date in a laboratory setting. 1,2 And yet, once people are in established relationships, they are happier with those relationships when their partners match their ideals. 2,3,4 In other words, we all know what we want in a romantic partner, but we often fail to choose dating partners based on those preferences. This is despite the fact that choosing romantic partners who possess the traits that we prefer would probably make us happier in the long run. Clearly, the human mate selection process and our decisions about our partners have room for improvement.


My colleagues and I recently explored one way in which dating decisions may get derailed: concern for the other person's feelings. In order for a person to select only dates who meet their ideals, they have to filter out all the other available and interested dates who don't meet their ideals. However, human beings are prosocial animals: we don't like to reject people, and we don't like to cause other people pain. Rejecting undesirable dates may be quite difficult to do -- perhaps harder than we anticipate -- and this desire to avoid hurting others' feelings may be part of what leads people to start to build relationships with people who don't meet their ideals.


I tested these hypotheses with Dr. Geoff MacDonald and Dr. Rimma Teper. 5 In two studies, we brought single undergraduate students into the lab and presented them with a dating profile that ostensibly belonged to a fellow student. In Study 1, we made the potential date seem undesirable to the participants by pairing it with an unattractive photograph. In Study 2, for each participant, we paired the dating profile with traits that the participant had previously identified as personal dealbreakers. For example, if the participant had said in a previous survey that they would never date a person who was highly religious, or a smoker, or a person who voted conservative, then the dating profile they received would indicate that the potential date was a highly religious conservative smoker. Alternatively, the potential date could be made to be an atheist liberal non-smoker, or some other combination of traits -- whatever was inconsistent with the participant's personal preferences.


In both studies, some participants were randomly assigned to the hypothetical condition. These participants were told that we had been unable to schedule anyone for their session, and so the dating profile was from a previous session. They were asked to imagine that the person was in the lab and ready to meet them, and tell us what they would choose to do if the situation were real. Other participants were randomly assigned to the real condition, in which they were led to believe that the potential date was actually in the lab and ready to meet them.


We found that people were much more likely to agree to a date with the undesirable suitor when they believed the situation was real rather than hypothetical. In Study 1, only 16 percent of people in the hypothetical condition predicted that they would agree to a date with the unattractive potential partner, but 37 percent of participants in the real condition actually agreed to a date with the unattractive potential partner.


This article was originally written for Science of Relationships: a website about the psychology of relationships that is written by active researchers and professors in the field.

Similarly, in Study 2, 46 percent of participants in the hypothetical condition predicted that they would agree to a date with the incompatible potential partner; however, 74 percent of participants in the real condition agreed to a date with the incompatible potential partner. In both cases, we found that our effects were partially explained by concern for the potential partner. When people thought that the potential partner was actually in the lab, they were motivated to avoid hurting that person's feelings -- significantly more so than the people who were only imagining the scenario -- and that helped to explain their willingness to agree to go on a date with this person.


This research suggests that rejecting dates from people who don't meet our ideals is easier said than done. Although we like to think of ourselves as being choosy and selective, when actually faced with an opportunity to go on a date with someone, it's difficult to turn the date down because doing so might cause the person pain. In future work, my colleagues and I will investigate what the long-term consequences of this phenomenon might be. How far might people be willing to go to avoid rejecting someone?


On the one hand, incompatible pairings might just fizzle out after a date or two, as the would-be partner's flaws become increasingly obvious. On the other hand, research suggests that our empathy for a person tends to grow as we get closer to them. 6 So our motivation to avoid rejecting a person may only strengthen, not weaken, as the new relationship develops. Either way, these findings suggest that in order to wind up with an ideal match, it's important that we overcome the pain of rejection -- not just the pain of experiencing rejection, but the pain of inflicting it as well.


1. Eastwick, P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2008). Sex differences in mate preferences revisited: Do people know what they initially desire in a romantic partner? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 245-264.


2. Eastwick, P. W., Finkel, E. J., & Eagly, A. H. (2011). When and why do ideal partner preferences affect the process of initiating and maintaining romantic relationships? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 1012-1032.


3. Fletcher, G. J. O., Simpson, J. A., & Thomas, G. (2000). Ideals, perceptions, and evaluations in early relationship development. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 933-940.


4. Eastwick, P. W., Luchies, L. B., Finkel, E. J., & Hunt, L. L. (2014). The predictive validity of ideal partner preferences: A review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 623-655.


5. Joel, S., Teper, R., & MacDonald, G. (in press). People overestimate their willingness to reject potential romantic partners by overlooking their concern for others. Psychological Science.


6. Loewenstein, G., & Small, D. (2007). The scarecrow and the tin man: The vicissitudes of human sympathy and caring. Review of General Psychology, 112, 112-126.


Entities 0 Name: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Count: 3 1 Name: P. W. Count: 2 2 Name: Finkel Count: 2 3 Name: P. W. , & Finkel Count: 1 4 Name: G. J. O. Count: 1 5 Name: E. J. , & Eagly Count: 1 6 Name: Joel Count: 1 7 Name: Teper Count: 1 8 Name: Dr. Geoff MacDonald Count: 1 9 Name: Luchies Count: 1 10 Name: L. B. Count: 1 11 Name: General Psychology Count: 1 12 Name: Loewenstein Count: 1 13 Name: Dr. Rimma Teper Count: 1 14 Name: Psychological Science Count: 1 15 Name: Simpson Count: 1 16 Name: Fletcher Count: 1 Related 0 Url: http://ift.tt/1vyaiWf Title: ​Ask Dr. Nerdlove: Dating On Hard Mode Description: Hello Internet! Welcome to Ask Dr. NerdLove, the only dating advice column that helps you answer the eternal call of Looking For Group for the epic endgame raid of life. This week, we handle the tricky question of dating on hard mode.

Post a Comment for "The Science of Breakups: Why Calling Things Off Is Easier Said Than Done - Huffington Post"